Neurociencia cognitivadiferencias entre distintas poblaciones en tareas de toma de decisiones

  1. Juárez Ramos, Verónica
Dirigida por:
  1. Emilio Gómez Milán Director/a

Universidad de defensa: Universidad de Granada

Fecha de defensa: 05 de mayo de 2014

Tribunal:
  1. Miguel Pérez García Presidente/a
  2. José Luis Mata Martín Secretario/a
  3. Simonetta Cutini Vocal
  4. Gustavo Adolfo Reyes del Paso Vocal
  5. Antonio González Hernández Vocal

Tipo: Tesis

Resumen

El cerebro es un dispositivo de contrastes de hipótesis, que confronta de modo continuo el procesamiento de abajo-arriba (o entrada sensorial) con el procesamiento de arriba-abajo (expectativas previas o formulación de hipótesis). De manera que de modo continuo estas dos corrientes de información se comparan realizando un contraste de hipótesis. El objetivo principal de esta tesis era mostrar que el ajuste a la realidad o la objetividad (congruencia entre interpretaciones y hechos) dista de ser perfecto en los seres humanos. Podemos clasificar a los grupos humanos en función de la magnitud de este desajuste, por eso estudiamos las diferencias existentes entre distintas poblaciones en la toma de decisiones en función de variables demográficas muy generales: género, edad, creencia religiosa, ideología política o rasgos de personalidad como la esquizotipia; aunque centrándonos en el estudio de los sesgos cognitivos (tareas sobre salto a conclusiones, sobre sesgo contra la evidencia desconfirmatoria o a favor de la evidencia confirmatoria, toma de riesgos, impulsividad¿), pero concentrándonos más detenidamente en el estudio del sesgo cognitivo ¿Salto a conclusiones¿, a través, de una tarea modificada con respecto a la tarea original llamada ¿Drawing Decision Task¿ (DDT o PDT en español) de Moritz y Woodward (2006). Además de los parámetros clásicos (índice de plausibilidad y de llegada a una decisión, BADE/BACE), esta división nos permitió calcular nuevos parámetros, en concreto, el número de respuestas correctas en la etapa 8 y la sensibilidad a la retroalimentación. Nosotros elegimos esta tarea porque nos daba una gran oportunidad de estudiar más exhaustivamente estos sesgos y entender mejor los mecanismos subyacentes a estos. En nuestra investigación el sesgo cognitivo salto a conclusiones se ha visto relacionado con el rasgo de personalidad esquizotipia, con el sexo masculino, creer en Dios y tener una orientación política conservadora. De modo que las personas que más saltan a conclusiones o muestran más sesgos cognitivos son hombres, creyentes de la tercera edad y alta esquizotipia. El perfil mayoritario de los políticos, empresarios, médicos, científicos, jueces¿Además, con respecto a los sesgos cognitivos BADE (sesgo en contra de la evidencia desconfirmatoria) y BACE (sesgo en contra de la evidencia confirmatoria) se ha podido establecer una relación entre ideología conservadora, alta esquizotipia, y la creencia en Dios. Referencias Amodio D.M., Jost J.T., Master S.L., Yee C.M., 2007. Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience. 10(10) 1246-47. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:7¿15. Bechara A., Tranel D., Damasio H., Damasio A.R., (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 6:215¿225. Bechara, A., Damasio H., Damasio A.R., (2000). Emotion, decision making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295-307. Byrnes J.P., Miller D.C., Schafer W.D., (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 125(3) 367-383. Bolla, K. I., Eldreth, D. A., Matochik, J. A., & Cadet, J. L. (2004). Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neural correlates. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1226-1232. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh083 Bonanno, G. A., & Jost, J. T., (2006). Conservative shift among highexposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 28, 311¿323. Block, J., & Block, J. H. (2006). Nursery school personality and political orientation two decades later. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 734¿749. Buchy, L., Woodward, T.S., Liotti, M., 2007. A cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) is associated with schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 90, 334-337. Carney, D., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology. 29, 807¿840. Chirumbolo, A., Areni, A., & Sensales, G. (2004). Need for cognitive closure and politics: Voting, political attitudes and attributional style. International Journal of Psychology. 39, 245¿253. . Damasio AR (1994) Descartes¿ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam. Davids, C., Patte, K., Tweed S., Curis, C., (2007). Personality traits associated with decision-making deficits. Personality and Individual Differences. 42, 279-290. Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2005). The ability to decide advantageously declines prematurely in some normal older persons. Neuropsychologia. 43, 1099-1106. Dougherty, M. R., Gettys, C. F., & Ogden, E. E. (1999). MINERVA-DM: A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. Psychological Review. 106, 180-209. Fear C.F., Healy D., (1997). Probabilistic reasoning in obsessive-compulsive and delusional disorders. Psychological Medicine. 27, 199-208. Federico, C. M., Hunt, C. V., & Ergun, D. (2009). Political expertise, social worldviews, and ideology: Translating ``competitive jungles¿¿ and ``dangerous worlds¿¿ into ideological reality. Social Justice Research, 22, 259¿279. Freeman, D., Pugh, K., Garety, P., 2008. Jumping to conclusions and paranoid ideation in the general population. Schizophria Research. 102 (1¿3), 254¿260. Hatemi, P., et al. (2011). A genome-wide analysis of liberal and nconservative political attitudes. Journal of Politics, 73, 271¿285. Harris, S., Kaplan, J. T., Curiel, A., Bookheimer, S. Y., Iacoboni, M., & Cohen, M. S., (2009). The neural correlates of religious and nonreligious belief. PLoS ONE. 4, e0007272. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007272. Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive bias. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. pp. 724¿746. Huq S.F., Garety P.A., Hemsley D.R., (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 40 (4) 801-12. Inzlicht M., McGregor I., Hirsh J.B., Nash K., (2009). Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychological Science. 20(3) 385-393. Jost, J.T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A.W. & Sulloway, F.J., 2003. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition Psychological Bulletin. 129, 339¿375. Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist. 61, 651-670. Jost, J.T., Amodio D.M., 2012. Political ideology as motivated social cognition. Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Motivation and Emotion. 36(1), 55-64.. Kahneman, Daniel (2011) Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux ISBN 978-0-374-27563-1 Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. En T. Gilovich, D. Griffin & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and Biases (pp. 49¿81). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D. (2002). Maps of bounded rationality: a perspective on intuitive judgment and choice. Prize Lecture, December 8. Princeton University, Department of Psychology, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA. Kaplan, J. T., Freedman, J., & Iacoboni, M. (2007). Us vs. them: Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural response to faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia, 45, 55¿64. Knutson, K., Wood, J. N., Spampinato, M. V., & Grafman, J. (2006).Politics on the brain: An fMRI investigation. Social Neuroscience. 1, 25¿40. Krain AL., Wilson A.M., Arbuckle R., Castellanos F.X., Milham M.P.,(2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making. Neuroimage. Aug 1;32(1):477-84. Epub 2006 Apr 24.. Lee G., Barrowclough C., Lobban F., 2011. The influence of positive affect on jumping to conclusions I delusional thinking. Personality and Individual Differences. 50, 717-722. Lincoln T.M., Ziegler M., Mehl S., Rief W., 2011. The jumping to conclusions bias in delusions: specificity and changeability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 119 (1) 40-9. Matthews, M., Levin, S., & Sidanius, J. (2009). A longitudinal test of the model of political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Political Psychology, 30, 921¿936. Mittal M., Vyas R.K., (2011). A study of psychological reasons for gender differences in preferences for risk and investment decision making. The IUP Journal of Behavioral Finance. 3(3) 45-60. McKay R., Langdon R., Coltheart M., (2006). Need for closure, jumping to conclusions, and decisiveness in delusion-prone individuals. The Journal of Nervous and mental disease. 194 (6) 422-6. Mondak, Jeffery J., and Karen D. Halperin. (2008). A framework for the study of personality and political behaviour. British Journal of Political Science. 38: 335¿62. Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., (2004). Plausibility Judgment in Schizophrenic Patients: Evidence for a Liberal Acceptance Bias. German Journal of Psychiatry. 7, 66-74. Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., 2005. Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non -delusional schizophrenic patients. British Journal Clinical Psychology. 44 (2), 193¿207. Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., 2006. A generalized bias against disconfirmatory evidence in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. 142 (2¿3), 157¿165. Moritz S., Woodward T.S., Lambert M., 2007. Under what circumstances do patients with schizophrenia jump to conclusions? A liberal acceptance account. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 46(Pt 2):127-37. Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., 2007. A cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence . (BADE) is associated with schizotypy. Shizophrenia Research. 90 (334-337). Olson, J. M., Vernon, P. A., Harris, J. A., & Jang, K. L. (2001). The heritability of attitudes: A study of twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 845¿860. Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, M. S., Hatemi, P. K., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667¿1670. Pfister, H.-R., & Böhm, G. (2008). The multiplicity of emotions: A framework of emotional functions in decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 5¿17. Rubio J.L., Ruíz- Veguilla M., Hernández L., Barrigón M.L., Salcedo M.D., Moreno, J.M., Gómez E., Moritz S., Ferrín M., (2011). Jumping to conclusions in psychosis: a faulty appraisal. Shizophrenia Research. 133 (1-3) 199-204. Shook, N. J., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Political ideology, exploration of novel stimuli, and attitude formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 45, 995¿998. Sidanius, J. (1978). Intolerance of ambiguity and socio-politico ideology: A multidimensional analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology. 8, 215¿235. Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99 -118. doi:10.2307/1884852. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645¿665. Taylor, P., Funk, C., & Craighill, P. (2006). Are we happy yet? http://pewresearch.org/assets/social/pdf/AreWeHappyYet.pdf Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). "Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.". Sciences 185: 1124¿1131. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1982). Evidential impact of base rates. En Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. & Tversky, A. (Eds). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases (pp.153¿160). New York: Cambridge University Press. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453¿458. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). "Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgement". Psychological Review 90: 293¿315. Webster, D., & Kruglanski, A. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049¿1062.