La precisión del recuerdo de patrones de juego en jugadores infantiles (sub-13 años) de balonmano

  1. Vicente Luis del Campo 1
  2. Cristóbal González Portillo 1
  3. Jesús Morenas Martín 1
  1. 1 Universidad de Extremadura. Laboratorio de Aprendizaje y Control Motor, Facultad de Ciencias del Deporte
Journal:
E-Balonmano.com: Revista de Ciencias del Deporte

ISSN: 1885-7019

Year of publication: 2020

Volume: 16

Issue: 3

Pages: 159-166

Type: Article

More publications in: E-Balonmano.com: Revista de Ciencias del Deporte

Abstract

The aim of the study was to address whether the skill to recall play patterns in handball was influenced by the temporal period in which those actions were perceived by the players. The sample of participants was seven U-13 federated handball players. The participants had to observe different play actions of a competition match played previously, and then, they had to judge how these actions ended. The recall tests were performed 1 day (1st test), 15 days (2nd test), and 30 days (3rd test) after the match. Specifically, the players perceived a set of 18 representative play actions, including 6 trials for each test. They had 30 seconds to recall how the play actions finished, selecting one option between several ones available on an observation sheet created specifically for this study. The players decreased their performance recalling the end of the play patterns as more time elapsed since the date that they played the match and the recall test. For example, their recall was significantly worse in the third test compared to the second and the first tests.

Bibliographic References

  • Allard, F., Graham, S., & Paarsalu, M. L. (1980). Perception in sport: basketball. Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 14-21.
  • Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M. W., & Anderson, A. C. (2009). Memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Bishara, A. J., & Hittner, J. B. (2012). Testing the significance of a correlation with nonnormal data: comparison of Pearson, Spearman, transformation, and resampling approaches. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 399-417. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028087.
  • Cañal-Bruland, R., & Schmidt, M. (2009). Response Bias in Judging Deceptive Movements, Acta Psychologica, 130, 235-240.
  • Chase, W.G., & Simon, A. S. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55-81.
  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363-406.
  • Farrow, D., & Reid, M. (2012). The contribution of situational probability information to anticipatory skill. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 15, 368-373.
  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 141, 2-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024338.
  • Funahashi, S. (2006). Prefrontal cortex and working memory processes. Neuroscience, 139(1), 251-261.
  • Gorman, A. D., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2012). Classical pattern recall tests and the prospective nature of expert performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1151-1160.
  • Gorman, A. D., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2015). Evidence of different underlying processes in pattern recall and decision-making. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(9), 1813-1831.
  • Murphy, C., Jackson, P., Robin, C., Roca, A., & Williams, A. M. (2015). Cognitive processes underlying anticipation in a context-oriented task. Journal of Sport Exercise and Psychology, S37, 53.
  • North, J. S., & Williams, A. M. (2007). The processes underpinning skilled recognition and anticipation in soccer. In P. Beek, and R. van de Langenberg (Eds), The European Workshop on Movement Science 2007 (p. 42). Koln: Sportverlag Strauss.
  • North, J. S., & Williams, A. M. (2008). The critical time period for information extraction when identifying patterns of play in soccer. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79(2), 268-273.
  • North, J. S., Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., Ward, P., & Ericsson, K. A. (2009). Some Constraints on Recognition Performance in Soccer. In D. Araujo, H. Ripoll, and M. Raab (Eds.), Perspectives on Cognition and Action in Sport (pp. 95-107). Science Publishers.
  • Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Greaves, M. (2012). Modelling working memory: An interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 779–819. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0272-4.
  • Pizzera, A., & Raab, M. (2012). Perceptual judgments of sports officials are influenced by their motor and visual experience. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 24(1), 59-72.
  • Raab, M. (2003). Decision making in sports: Implicit and explicit learning is affected by complexity of situation. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 406-433.
  • Raab, M., & Farrow, D. (2015). Examining the stability and specificity of pattern recall in team handball. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 46, 513-527.
  • Roca, A., Ford, P. R., McRobert, A. P., & Williams, A. M. (2011). Identifying the processes underpinning anticipation and decision-making in a dynamic time-constrained task. Cognitive Processing, 12, 301–310.
  • Schapschröer, M., Baker, J. & Schorer, J. (2016). Exploring the interaction of physical exercise load and pattern recall performance in female handball players. Experimental Brain Research, 234(2), 1713. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4584-x
  • Van Maarseeven, M. J. J., Oudejans, R. R. D., Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2015). Pattern recall skills of talented soccer players: Two new methods applied. Human Movement Science, 41, 59-75.
  • Ward, P., Hodges, N. J., Starkes, J. L., & Williams, A. M. (2007). The road to excellence: deliberate practice and the development of expertise. High Ability Studies, 18, 119-153.
  • Ward, P., & Williams, A. M. (2003). Perceptual and cognitive skill development in soccer: the multidimensional nature of expert performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25(1), 93-111.
  • Williams, A. M. (2000). Perceptual skill in soccer: Implications for talent identification and development. Journal of Sport Sciences, 18, 737-750.
  • Williams, A. M., & Davids, K. (1995). Declarative knowledge in sport: A byproduct of experience or characteristic of expertise? Journal of Sport and Expertise Psychology, 17, 259-275.
  • Williams, A. M., Davids, K., Burwitz, L. and Williams, J. G. (1993). Cognitive knowledge and soccer performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 579-593.
  • Williams, A. M., Davids, K., Burwitz, L., & Williams, J. G. (1994). Visual search strategies in experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 127-135.
  • Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual Perception & Action in Sport. London: E&FN Sport.
  • Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., North, J. S., & Barton, G. (2006). Perceiving patterns of play in dynamic sport tasks: Identifying the essential information underlying skilled performance. Perception, 35, 317-332.
  • Williams, A. M., & North, J. S. (2009). Some constraints on recognition performance in soccer. In D. Araujo, H. Ripoll, and M. Raab (Eds.), Perspectives on Cognition and Action in Sport (pp. 95-107).
  • Williams, A. M., Ward, P. (2003). Perceptual expertise: development in sport. In J. L. Starkes, and A. Ericsson (Eds), Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 219-247). Champaign, IL, Human Kinetics.